1. Po raz pierwszy odwiedzasz EDU. LEARN

    Odwiedzasz EDU.LEARN

    Najlepszym sposobem na naukę języka jest jego używanie. W EDU.LEARN znajdziesz interesujące teksty i videa, które dadzą Ci taką właśnie możliwość. Nie przejmuj się - nasze filmiki mają napisy, dzięki którym lepiej je zrozumiesz. Dodatkowo, po kliknięciu na każde słówko, otrzymasz jego tłumaczenie oraz prawidłową wymowę.

    Nie, dziękuję
  2. Mini lekcje

    Podczas nauki języka bardzo ważny jest kontekst. Zdjęcia, przykłady użycia, dialogi, nagrania dźwiękowe - wszystko to pomaga Ci zrozumieć i zapamiętać nowe słowa i wyrażenia. Dlatego stworzyliśmy Mini lekcje. Są to krótkie lekcje, zawierające kontekstowe slajdy, które zwiększą efektywność Twojej nauki. Są cztery typy Mini lekcji - Gramatyka, Dialogi, Słówka i Obrazki.

    Dalej
  3. Wideo

    Ćwicz język obcy oglądając ciekawe filmiki. Wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na filmik. Nie martw się, obok każdego z nich są napisy. A może wcale nie będą Ci one potrzebne? Spróbuj!

    Dalej
  4. Teksty

    Czytaj ciekawe artykuły, z których nauczysz się nowych słówek i dowiesz więcej o rzeczach, które Cię interesują. Podobnie jak z filmikami, możesz wybrać temat oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknąć na wybrany artykuł. Nasz interaktywny słownik pomoże Ci zrozumieć nawet trudne teksty, a kontekst ułatwi zapamiętanie słówek. Dodatkowo, każdy artykuł może być przeczytany na głos przez wirtualnego lektora, dzięki czemu ćwiczysz słuchanie i wymowę!

    Dalej
  5. Słowa

    Tutaj możesz znaleźć swoją listę "Moje słówka", czyli funkcję wyszukiwania słówek - a wkrótce także słownik tematyczny. Do listy "Moje słówka" możesz dodawać słowa z sekcji Videa i Teksty. Każde z słówek dodanych do listy możesz powtórzyć później w jednym z naszych ćwiczeń. Dodatkowo, zawsze możesz iść do swojej listy i sprawdzić znaczenie, wymowę oraz użycie słówka w zdaniu. Użyj naszej wyszukiwarki słówek w części "Słownictwo", aby znaleźć słowa w naszej bazie.

    Dalej
  6. Lista tekstów

    Ta lista tekstów pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Teksty". Wybierz poziom trudności oraz temat, a następnie artykuł, który Cię interesuje. Kiedy już zostaniesz do niego przekierowany, kliknij na "Play", jeśli chcesz, aby został on odczytany przez wirtualnego lektora. W ten sposób ćwiczysz umiejętność słuchania. Niektóre z tekstów są szczególnie interesujące - mają one odznakę w prawym górnym rogu. Koniecznie je przeczytaj!

    Dalej
  7. Lista Video

    Ta lista filmików pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Video". Podobnie jak w przypadku Tekstów, najpierw wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na wybrane video. Te z odznaką w prawym górnym rogu są szczególnie interesujące - nie przegap ich!

    Dalej
  8. Dziękujemy za skorzystanie z przewodnika!

    Teraz już znasz wszystkie funkcje EDU.LEARN! Przygotowaliśmy do Ciebie wiele artykułów, filmików oraz mini lekcji - na pewno znajdziesz coś, co Cię zainteresuje!

    Teraz zapraszamy Cię do zarejestrowania się i odkrycia wszystkich możliwości portalu.

    Dziękuję, wrócę później
  9. Lista Pomocy

    Potrzebujesz z czymś pomocy? Sprawdź naszą listę poniżej:
    Nie, dziękuję

Już 62 429 użytkowników uczy się języków obcych z Edustation.

Możesz zarejestrować się już dziś i odebrać bonus w postaci 10 monet.

Jeżeli chcesz się dowiedzieć więcej o naszym portalu - kliknij tutaj

Jeszcze nie teraz

lub

Poziom:

Wszystkie

Nie masz konta?

3. Introduction to Psychology: Foundations: Skinner


Poziom:

Temat: Edukacja

Professor Paul Bloom: I actually want to begin by
going back to Freud and hitting a couple of loose ends.
There was a point in my lecture on Wednesday where I skipped
over some parts. I said, "We don't have time for
this" and I just whipped past it.
And I couldn't sleep over the weekend.
I've been tormented. I shouldn't have skipped that
and I want to hit--Let me tell you why I skipped it.
The discussion I skipped was the discussion of why we would
have an unconscious at all. So, I was talking about the
scientifically respectable ideas of Freud and I want to talk
about some new ideas about why there could be an unconscious.
Now, the reason why I skipped it is I'm not sure this is the
best way to look at the question.
As we will learn throughout the course, by far the vast majority
of what our brains do, the vast majority of what our
minds do, is unconscious and we're unaware of it.
So the right question to ask may not be, "Why are some things
unconscious?" but rather, why is this tiny
subset of mental life--why is this conscious?
On the other hand, these claims about the utility
of unconsciousness, I think, are provocative and
interesting. So I just wanted to quickly
share them with you. So, the question is,
from an evolutionary standpoint, "Why would an
unconscious evolve?" And an answer that some
psychologists and biologists have given is deception.
So, most animals do some deception.
And deception defined broadly is simply to act or be in some
way that fools others into believing or thinking or
responding to something that's false.
There's physical examples of deception.
When threatened, chimpanzees--their hair stands
up on end and that makes them look bigger to fool others to
thinking they're more dangerous than they are.
There's an angler fish at the bottom of the ocean that has a
rod sticking up from the top of its head with a lure to capture
other fish – to fool them in thinking that this is something
edible and then to themselves be devoured.
But humans, primates in general but particularly humans,
are masters of deception. We use our minds and our
behaviors and our actions continually to try to trick
people into believing what's not true.
We try to trick people, for instance,
into believing that we're tougher, smarter,
sexier, more reliable, more trustworthy and so on,
than we really are. And a large part of social
psychology concerns the way in which we present ourselves to
other people so as to make the maximally positive impression
even when that impression isn't true.
At the same time, though, we've also evolved very
good lie detection mechanisms. So not only is there
evolutionary pressure for me to lie to you, for me to persuade
you for instance, that if we're going to have
a--if you are threatening me don't threaten me,
I am not the sort of man you could screw around with.
But there's evolutionary pressure for you to look and
say, "No. You are the sort of man you
could screw around with. I can tell."
So how do you become a good liar?
And here's where the unconscious comes in.
The hypothesis is: the best lies are lies we tell
ourselves. You're a better liar,
more generally, if you believe the lie that
you're telling. This could be illustrated with
a story about Alfred Hitchcock. The story goes--He hated
working with child actors but he often had to.
And the story goes--He was dealing with a child actor who
simply could not cry. And, finally frustrated,
Hitchcock went to the actor, leaned over,
whispered in his ear, "Your parents have left you and
they're never coming back." The kid burst into tears.
Hitchcock said, "Roll ‘em" and filmed the
kid. And the kid,
if you were to see him, you'd say, "That's--Boy,
he's--he really looks as if he's sad" because he was.
If I had a competition where I'd give $100,000 to the person
who looks the most as if they are in pain,
it is a very good tactic to take a pen and jam it into your
groin because you will look extremely persuasively as if you
are in pain. If I want to persuade you that
I love you, would never leave you, you can trust me with
everything, it may be a superb tactic for me to believe it.
And so, this account of the evolution of the unconscious is
that certain motivations and goals,
particularly sinister ones, are better made to be
unconscious because if a person doesn't know they have them they
will not give them away. And this is something I think
we should return to later on when we talk about social
interaction and social relationships.
One other thing on Freud--just a story of the falsification of
Freud. I was taking my younger child
home from a play date on Sunday and he asked me out of the blue,
"Why can't you marry your mother or your father?"
Now, that's actually a difficult question to ask--to
answer for a child, but I tried my best to give him
an answer. And then I said--then I thought
back on the Freud lecture and so I asked him, "If you could marry
anybody you want, who would it be?"
imagining he'd make explicit the Oedipal complex and name his
mother. Instead, he paused for a moment
and said, "I would marry a donkey and a big bag of
peanuts." [laughter]
Both his parents are psychologists and he hates these
questions and at times he just screws around with us.
[laughter] Okay. Last class I started with Freud
and now I want to turn to Skinner.
And the story of Skinner and science is somewhat different
from the story of Freud. Freud developed and championed
the theory of psychoanalysis by himself.
It is as close as you could find in science to a solitary
invention. Obviously, he drew upon all
sorts of sources and predecessors but psychoanalysis
is identified as Freud's creation.
Behaviorism is different. Behaviorism is a school of
thought that was there long before Skinner,
championed by psychologists like John Watson,
for instance. Skinner came a bit late into
this but the reason why we've heard of Skinner and why Skinner
is so well known is he packaged these notions.
He expanded upon them; he publicized them;
he developed them scientifically and presented
them both to the scientific community and to the popular
community and sociologically in the 1960s and 1970s.
In the United States, behaviorism was incredibly well
known and so was Skinner. He was the sort of person you
would see on talk shows. His books were bestsellers.
Now, at the core of behaviorism are three extremely radical and
interesting views. The first is a strong emphasis
on learning. The strong view of behaviorism
is everything you know, everything you are,
is the result of experience. There's no real human nature.
Rather, people are infinitely malleable.
There's a wonderful quote from John Watson and in this quote
John Watson is paraphrasing a famous boast by the Jesuits.
The Jesuits used to claim, "Give me a child until the age
of seven and I'll show you the man,"
that they would take a child and turn him into anything they
wanted. And Watson expanded on this
boast, Give me a dozen healthy
infants, well-formed and my own specified world to bring them up
and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train them to
become any type of specialist I might select--doctor,
lawyer, artist, merchant, chief,
and yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his
talents, penchants, tendencies,
abilities, vocations and race of his ancestors.
Now, you could imagine--You could see in this a tremendous
appeal to this view because Watson has an extremely
egalitarian view in a sense. If there's no human nature,
then there's no sense in which one group of humans by dint of
their race or their sex could be better than another group.
And Watson was explicit. None of those facts about
people will ever make any difference.
What matters to what you are is what you learn and how you're
treated. And so, Watson claimed he could
create anybody in any way simply by treating them in a certain
fashion.
A second aspect of behaviorism was anti-mentalism.
And what I mean by this is the behaviorists were obsessed with
the idea of doing science and they felt,
largely in reaction to Freud, that claims about internal
mental states like desires, wishes, goals,
emotions and so on, are unscientific.
These invisible, vague things can never form the
basis of a serious science. And so, the behaviorist
manifesto would then be to develop a science without
anything that's unobservable and instead use notions like
stimulus and response and reinforcement and punishment and
environment that refer to real world and tangible events.
Finally, behaviorists believed there were no interesting
differences across species. A behaviorist might admit that
a human can do things that a rat or pigeon couldn't but a
behaviorist might just say, "Look.
Those are just general associative powers that differ"
or they may even deny it. They might say,
"Humans and rats aren't different at all.
It's just humans tend to live in a richer environment than
rats." From that standpoint,
from that theoretical standpoint, comes a
methodological approach which is,
if they're all the same then you could study human learning
by studying nonhuman animals. And that's a lot of what they
did. Okay.
I'm going to frame my introduction--my discussion of
behaviors in terms of the three main learning principles that
they argue can explain all of human mental life,
all of human behavior. And then, I want to turn to
objections to behaviorism but these three principles are
powerful and very interesting.
The first is habituation. This is the very simplest form
of learning. And what this is is technically
described as a decline in the tendency to respond to stimuli
that are familiar due to repeated exposure.
"Hey!" "Hey!"
The sudden noise startles but as it--as you hear it a second
time it startles less. The third time is just me being
goofy. It's just--It's--You get used
to things. And this, of course,
is common enough in everyday life.
We get used to the ticking of a clock or to noise of traffic but
it's actually a very important form of learning because imagine
life without it. Imagine life where you never
got used to anything, where suddenly somebody steps
forward and waves their hand and you'd go,
"Woah," and then they wave their hand again and you'd go,
"Whoah," and you keep--[laughter]
And there's the loud ticking of a clock and you say,
"Hmmm." And that's not the way animals
or humans work. You get used to things.
And it's actually critically important to get used to things
because it's a useful adaptive mechanism to keep track on new
events and objects. It's important to notice
something when it's new because then you have to decide whether
it's going to harm you, how to deal with it,
to attend to it, but you can't keep on noticing
it. And, in fact,
you should stop noticing it after it's been in the
environment for long enough. So, this counts as learning
because it happens through experience.
It's a way to learn through experience, to change your way
of thinking through experience. And also, it's useful because
harmful stimuli are noticed but when something has shown itself
to be part of the environment you don't notice it anymore.
The existence of habituation is important for many reasons.
One thing it's important for is clever developmental
psychologists have used habituation as a way to study
people, creatures who can't talk like
nonhuman animals, and young babies.
And when I talk on Wednesday about developmental psychology
I'll show different ways in which psychologists have used
habituation to study the minds of young babies.
The second sort of learning is known as classical conditioning.
And what this is in a very general sense is the learning of
an association between one stimulus and another stimulus,
where stimulus is a technical term meaning events in the
environment like a certain smell or sound or sight.
It was thought up by Pavlov. This is Pavlov's famous dog and
it's an example of scientific serendipity.
Pavlov, when he started this research, had no interest at all
in learning. He was interested in saliva.
And to get saliva he had to have dogs.
And he had to attach something to dogs so that their saliva
would pour out so he could study saliva.
No idea why he wanted to study saliva, but he then discovered
something. What he would do is he'd put
food powder in the dog's mouth to generate saliva.
But Pavlov observed that when somebody entered the room who
typically gave him the food powder,
the dog--the food powder saliva would start to come out.
And later on if you--right before or right during you give
the dog some food – you ping a bell – the bell will cause the
saliva to come forth. And, in fact,
this is the apparatus that he used for his research.
He developed the theory of classical conditioning by making
a distinction between two sorts of conditioning,
two sorts of stimulus response relationships.
One is unconditioned. An unconditioned is when an
unconditioned stimulus gives rise to an unconditioned
response. And this is what you start off
with. So, if somebody pokes you with
a stick and you say, "Ouch," because it hurts,
the poking and the "ouch" is an unconditioned stimulus causing
an unconditioned response. You didn't have to learn that.
When Pavlov put food powder in the dog's mouth and saliva was
generated, that's an unconditioned stimulus giving
rise to an unconditioned response.
But what happens through learning is that another
association develops – that between the conditioned stimulus
and the conditioned response. So when Pavlov,
for instance--Well, when Pavlov,
for instance, started before conditioning
there was simply an unconditioned stimulus,
the food in the mouth, and an unconditioned response,
saliva. The bell was nothing.
The bell was a neutral stimulus. But over and over again,
if you put the bell and the food together,
pretty soon the bell will generate saliva.
And now the bell--When--You start off with the unconditioned
stimulus, unconditioned response.
When the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus
are brought together over and over and over again,
pretty soon the conditioned stimulus gives rise to the
response. And now it's known as the
conditioned stimulus giving rise to the conditioned response.
This is discussed in detail in the textbook but I also--I'm
going to give you--Don't panic if you don't get it quite now.
I'm going to give you further and further examples.
So, the idea here is, repeated pairings of the
unconditioned stimulus and the conditioned stimulus will give
rise to the response. And there's a difference
between reinforced trials and unreinforced trials.
A reinforced trial is when the conditioned stimulus and the
unconditioned stimulus go together.
You're--and to put it in a crude way, you're teaching the
dog that the bell goes with the food.
An unreinforced trial is when you get the food without the
bell. You're not teaching the dog
this. And, in fact,
once you teach an animal something, if you stop doing the
teaching the response goes away and this is known as extinction.
But here's a graph. If you get--They really count
the number of cubic centimeters of saliva.
The dog is trained so that when the bell comes on--Actually,
I misframed it. I'll try again.
When the bell comes connected with food, there's a lot of
saliva. An unreinforced response is
when the bell goes on but there's no food.
So, it's--Imagine you're the dog.
So, you get food in your mouth, "bell, food,
bell, food," and now "bell." But next you get "bell,
bell, bell." You give it up.
You stop. You stop responding to the bell.
A weird thing which is discussed in the textbook is if
you wait a while and then you try it again with the bell after
a couple of hours, the saliva comes back.
This is known as spontaneous recovery.
So, this all seems a very technical phenomena related to
animals and the like but it's easy to see how it generalizes
and how it extends. One interesting notion is that
of stimulus generalization. And stimulus generalization is
the topic of one of your articles in The Norton
Reader, the one by Watson,
John Watson, the famous behaviorist,
who reported a bizarre experiment with a baby known as
Little Albert. And here's the idea.
Little Albert originally liked rats.
In fact, I'm going to show you a movie of Little Albert
originally liking rats.
See. He's okay.
No problem. Now, Watson did something
interesting. As Little Albert was playing
with the rat, "Oh, I like rats,
oh," Watson went behind the baby--this is the--it's in the
chapter--and banged the metal bar right here .
The baby, "Aah," screamed, started to sob.
Okay. What's the unconditioned
stimulus? Somebody.
The loud noise, the bar, the bang.
What's the unconditioned response?
Crying, sadness, misery. And as a result of this,
Little Albert grew afraid of the rat.
So there--what would be the conditioned stimulus?
The rat. What would be the conditioned
response? Fear.
Excellent. Moreover, this fear extended to
other things. So, this is a very weird and
unpersuasive clip. But the idea is--the clip is to
make the point that the fear will extend to a rabbit,
a white rabbit. So, the first part,
Little Albert's fine with the white rabbit.
The second part is after he's been conditioned and he's kind
of freaked out with the white rabbit.
The problem is in the second part they're throwing the rabbit
at him but now he's okay.
[laughter] Is the mic on? Oh.
This is fine. This is one of a long list of
experiments that we can't do anymore.
So, classical conditioning is more than a laboratory
phenomena. The findings of classical
conditioning have been extended and replicated in all sorts of
animals including crabs, fish, cockroaches and so on.
And it's been argued to be an extension of--it's argued to
underlie certain interesting aspects of human responses.
So, I have some examples here. One example is fear.
So, the Little Albert idea--The Little Albert experiment,
provides an illustration for how phobias could emerge.
Some proportion of people in this room have phobias.
Imagine you're afraid of dogs. Well, a possible story for
the--for why you became afraid of dogs is that one day a dog
came up and he was a neutral stimulus.
No problem. And all of a sudden he bit you.
Now the pain of a bite, being bit, and then the pain
and fear of that is an unconditioned stimulus,
unconditioned response. You're just born with that,
"ow." But the presence of the dog
there is a conditioned stimulus and so you grew to be afraid of
dogs. If you believe this,
this also forms the basis for ways for a theory of how you
could make phobias go away. How do you make conditioned
stimulus, conditioned response things go away?
Well, what you do is you extinguish them.
How do you extinguish them? Well, you show the thing that
would cause you to have the fear without the unconditioned
stimulus. Here's an illustration.
It's a joke. Sorry.
He's simultaneously confronting the fear of heights,
snakes, and the dark because he's trapped in that thing and
the logic is--the logic of--the logic is not bad.
He's stuck in there. Those are all the--his
conditioned stimulus. But nothing bad happens so his
fear goes away. The problem with this is while
he's stuck in there he has this screaming, horrific panic attack
and then it makes his fear much worse.
So, what they do now though, and we'll talk about this much
later in the course when we talk about clinical psychology--but
one cure for phobias does draw upon,
in a more intelligent way, the behaviorist literature.
So, the claim about a phobia is that there's a bad association
between, say dog and fear, or between airplanes or snakes
and some bad response. So, what they do is what's
called, "systematic desensitization," which is they
expose you to what causes you the fear but they relax you at
the same time so you replace the aversive classical conditioned
fear with something more positive.
Traditionally, they used to teach people
relaxation exercises but that proves too difficult.
So nowadays they just pump you full of some drug to get you
really happy and so you're really stoned out of your head,
you're and this isn't so bad. It's more complicated than that
but the notion is you can use these associative tools perhaps
to deal with questions about fear,
phobias and how they go away. Hunger.
We'll spend some time in this course discussing why we eat and
when we eat. And one answer to why we eat
and when we eat is that there's cues in the environment that are
associated with eating. And these cues generate hunger.
For those of you who are trying to quit smoking,
you'll notice that there's time--or to quit drinking
there's times of the day or certain activities that really
make you want to smoke or really make you want to drink.
And from a behaviorist point of view this is because of the
associative history of these things.
More speculatively, classical conditioning has been
argued to be implicated in the formation of sexual desire,
including fetishes. So a behaviorist story about
fetishes, for instance, is it's straightforward
classical conditioning. Just as your lover's caress
brings you to orgasm, your eyes happen to fall upon a
shoe. Through the simple tools of
classical conditioning then, the shoe becomes a conditioned
stimulus giving rise to the conditioned response of sexual
pleasure. This almost certainly is not
the right story but again, just as in phobias,
some ideas of classical conditioning may play some role
in determining what we like and what we don't like sexually.
And in fact, one treatment for pedophiles
and rapists involved controlled fantasies during masturbation to
shift the association from domination and violence,
for instance, to develop more positive
associations with sexual pleasure.
So the strong classical conditioning stories about
fetishes and fears sound silly and extreme and they probably
are but at the same time classical conditioning can be
used at least to shape the focus of our desires and of our
interests. Final thought actually is--Oh,
yeah. Okay.
So, what do we think about classical conditioning?
We talked about what habituation is for.
What's classical conditioning for?
Well, the traditional view is it's not for anything.
It's just association. So, what happens is the UCS and
the CS, the bell and the food, go together because they happen
at the same time. And so classical conditioning
should be the strongest when these two are simultaneous and
the response to one is the same as the response to the other.
This is actually no longer the mainstream view.
The mainstream view is now a little bit more interesting.
It's that what happens in classical conditioning is
preparation. What happens is you become
sensitive to a cue that an event is about to happen and that
allows you to prepare for the event.
This makes certain predictions. It predicts that the best
timing is when the conditioned stimulus, which is the signal,
comes before the unconditioned stimulus, which is what you have
to prepare for. And it says the conditioned
response may be different from the unconditioned response.
So, move away from food. Imagine a child who's being
beaten by his father. And when his father raises his
hand he flinches. Well, that's classical
conditioning. What happened in that case is
he has learned that the raising of a hand is a signal that he is
about to be hit and so he responds to that signal.
His flinch is not the same response that one would give if
one's hit. If you're hit, you don't flinch.
If you're hit, you might feel pain or bounce
back or something. Flinching is preparation for
being hit. And, in general,
the idea of what goes on in classical conditioning is that
the response is sort of a preparation.
The conditioned response is a preparation for the
unconditioned stimulus. Classical conditioning shows up
all over the place. As a final exercise,
and I had to think about it--Has anybody here seen the
movie "Clockwork Orange"? A lot of you.
It's kind of a shocking movie and unpleasant and very violent
but at its core one of the main themes is right out of Intro
Psych. It's classical conditioning.
And a main character, who is a violent murderer and
rapist, is brought in by some psychologists for some therapy.
And the therapy he gets is classical conditioning.
In particular, what happens is he is given a
drug that makes him violently ill, extremely nauseous.
And then his eyes are propped open and he's shown scenes of
violence. As a result of this sort of
conditioning, he then – when he experiences
real world violence – he responds with nausea and shock;
basically, training him to get away from these acts of
violence. In this example--Take a moment.
Don't say it aloud. Just take a moment.
What's the unconditioned stimulus?
Okay. Anybody, what's the
unconditioned stimulus? Somebody just say it.
The drug. What's the unconditioned
response? Nausea.
What's the conditioned stimulus? Violence.
What's the conditioned response? Perfect.
The third and final type of learning is known as operant
conditioning or instrumental conditioning.
And this is the thing, this is the theory championed
and developed most extensively by Skinner.
What this is is learning the relationships between what you
do and how successful or unsuccessful they are,
learning what works and what doesn't.
It's important. This is very different from
classical conditioning and one way to see how this is different
is for classical conditioning you don't do anything.
You could literally be strapped down and be immobile and these
connections are what you appreciate and you make
connections in your mind. Instrumental conditioning is
voluntary. You choose to do things and by
dint of your choices. Some choices become more
learned than others. So, the idea itself was
developed in the nicest form by Thorndike who explored how
animals learn. Remember behaviorists were
entirely comfortable studying animals and drawing
extrapolations to other animals and to humans.
So, he would put a cat in a puzzle box.
And the trick to a puzzle box is there's a simple way to get
out but you have to kind of pull on something,
some special lever, to make it pop open.
And Thorndike noted that cats do not solve this problem
through insight. They don't sit in the box for a
while and mull it over and then figure out how to do it.
Instead, what they do is they bounce all around doing
different things and gradually get better and better at it.
So, what they do is, the first time they might
scratch at the bars, push at the ceiling,
dig at the floor, howl, etc., etc.
And one of their behaviors is pressing the lever.
The lever gets them out of the box, but after more and more
trials they stopped scratching at the bars, pushing at the
ceiling and so on. They just pressed the lever.
And if you graph it, they gradually get better and
better. They throw out all of these
behaviors randomly. Some of them get reinforced and
those are the ones that survive and others don't get reinforced
and those are the ones that go extinct.
And it might occur to some of you that this seems to be an
analogy with the Darwinian theory of natural selection
where there's a random assortment of random mutations.
And sexual selections give rise to a host of organisms,
some of which survive and are fit and others which aren't.
And in fact, Skinner explicitly made the
analogy from the natural selection of species to the
natural selection of behavior. So this could be summarized as
the law of effect, which is a tendency to perform
– an action's increased if rewarded, weakened if it's not.
And Skinner extended this more generally.
So, to illustrate Skinnerian theory in operant conditioning
I'll give an example of training a pig.
So here is the idea. You need to train a pig and you
need to do so through operant conditioning.
So one of the things you want to do is--The pig is going to do
some things you like and some things you don't like.
And so what you want to do, basically drawing upon the law
of effect, is reinforce the pig for doing good things.
Suppose you want the pig to walk forward.
So, you reinforce the pig for walking forward and you punish
the pig for walking backward. And if you do that over the
fullness of time, your reinforcement and
punishment will give rise to a pig who walks forward.
There's two--One technical distinction that people love to
put on Intro Psych exams is that the difference between positive
reinforcement and negative reinforcement.
Reinforcement is something that makes the behavior increase.
Negative reinforcement is very different from punishment.
Negative reinforcement is just a type of reward.
The difference is in positive reinforcement you do something;
in negative reinforcement you take away something aversive.
So, imagine the pig has a heavy collar and to reward the pig for
walking forward you might remove the heavy collar.
So, these are the basic techniques to train an animal.
But it's kind of silly because suppose you want your pig to
dance. You don't just want your pig to
walk forward. You want your pig to dance.
Well, you can't adopt the policy of "I'm going to wait for
this pig to dance and when it does I'm going to reinforce it"
because it's going to take you a very long time.
Similarly, if you're dealing with immature humans and you
want your child to get you a beer,
you can't just sit, wait for the kid to give you a
beer and uncap the bottle and say,
"Excellent. Good.
Hugs." You've got to work your way to
it. And the act of working your way
to it is known as shaping. So, here is how to get a pig to
dance. You wait for the pig to do
something that's halfway close to dancing, like stumbling,
and you reward it. Then it does something else
that's even closer to dancing and you reward it.
And you keep rewarding it as it gets closer to closer.
Here's how to get your child to bring you some beer.
You say, "Johnny, could you go to the kitchen and
get me some beer?" And he walks to the kitchen and
then he forgets why he's there and you run out there.
"You're such a good kid. Congratulations.
Hugs." And then you get him to--and
then finally you get him to also open up the refrigerator and get
the beer, open the door,
get the--and in that way you can train creatures to do
complicated things. Skinner had many examples of
this. Skinner developed,
in World War II, a pigeon guided missile.
It was never actually used but it was a great idea.
And people, in fact--The history of the military in the
United States and other countries includes a lot of
attempts to get animals like pigeons or dolphins to do
interesting and deadly things through various training.
More recreational, Skinner was fond of teaching
animals to play Ping-Pong. And again, you don't teach an
animal to play Ping-Pong by waiting for it to play Ping-Pong
and then rewarding it. Rather, you reward
approximations to it. And basically,
there are primary reinforcers. There are some things pigs
naturally like, food for instance.
There are some things pigs actually automatically don't
like, like being hit or shocked. But in the real world when
dealing with humans, but even when dealing with
animals, we don't actually always use
primary reinforcers or negative reinforcers.
What we often use are things like--for my dog saying,
"Good dog." Now, saying "Good dog" is not
something your dog has been built, pre-wired,
to find pleasurable. But what happens is you can do
a two-step process. You can make "Good dog"
positive through classical conditioning.
You give the dog a treat and say, "Good dog."
Now the phrase "good dog" will carry the rewarding quality.
And you could use that rewarding quality in order to
train it. And through this way
behaviorists have developed token economies where they get
nonhuman animals to do interesting things for seemingly
arbitrary rewards like poker chips.
And in this way you can increase the utility and ease of
training. Finally, in the examples we're
giving, whenever the pig does something you like you reinforce
it. But that's not how real life
works. Real life for both humans and
animals involved cases where the reinforcement doesn't happen all
the time but actually happens according to different
schedules. And so, there is the
distinction between fixed schedules versus ratios –
variable schedules and ratio versus interval.
And this is something you could print out to look at.
I don't need to go over it in detail.
The difference between ratio is a reward every certain number of
times somebody does something. So, if every tenth time your
dog brought you the newspaper you gave it hugs and treats;
that's ratio. An interval is over a period of
time. So, if your dog gives you--if
your dog, I don't know, dances for an hour straight,
that would be an interval thing.
And fixed versus variable speaks to whether you give a
reward on a fixed schedule, every fifth time,
or variable, sometimes on the third time,
sometimes on the seventh time, and so on.
And these are--There are examples here and there's no
need to go over them. It's easy enough to think of
examples in real life. So, for example,
a slot machine is variable ratio.
It goes off after it's been hit a certain number of times.
It doesn't matter how long it takes you for--to do it.
It's the number of times you pull it down.
But it's variable because it doesn't always go off on the
thousandth time. You don't know.
It's unpredictable. The slot machine is a good
example of a phenomena known as the partial reinforcement
effect. And this is kind of neat.
It makes sense when you hear it but it's the sort of finding
that's been validated over and over again with animals and
nonhumans. Here's the idea.
Suppose you want to train somebody to do something and you
want the training such that they'll keep on doing it even if
you're not training them anymore,
which is typically what you want.
If you want that, the trick is don't reinforce it
all the time. Behaviors last longer if
they're reinforced intermittently and this is known
as "the partial reinforcement effect."
Thinking of this psychologically,
it's as if whenever you put something in a slot machine it
gave you money, then all of a sudden it
stopped. You keep on doing it a few
times but then you say, "Fine.
It doesn't work," but what if it gave you money one out of
every hundred times? Now you keep on trying and
because the reinforcement is intermittent you don't expect it
as much and so your behavior will persist across often a huge
amount of time. Here's a good example.
What's the very worst thing to do when your kid cries to go
into bed with you and you don't want him to go into bed with
you? Well, one--The worst thing to
do is for any--Actually, for any form of discipline with
a kid is to say, "No, absolutely not.
No, no, no, no." "Okay."
And then later on the kid's going to say,
"I want to do it again" and you say no and the kid keeps asking
because you've put it, well, put it as in a
psychological way, not the way the behaviorists
would put it. The kid knows okay,
he's not going to get it right away, he's going to keep on
asking. And so typically,
what you're doing inadvertently in those situations is you're
exploiting the partial reinforcement effect.
If I want my kid to do something, I should say yes one
out of every ten times. Unfortunately,
that's the evolution of nagging.
Because you nag, you nag, you nag,
the person says, "Fine, okay," and that
reinforces it. If Skinner kept the focus on
rats and pigeons and dogs, he would not have the impact
that he did but he argued that you could extend all of these
notions to humans and to human behavior.
So for an example, he argued that the prison
system needs to be reformed because instead of focusing on
notions of justice and retribution what we should do is
focus instead on questions of reinforcing good behaviors and
punishing bad ones. He argued for the notions of
operant conditioning to be extended to everyday life and
argued that people's lives would become fuller and more
satisfying if they were controlled in a properly
behaviorist way. Any questions about behaviorism?
What are your questions about behaviorism?
[laughter] Yes. Student:
[inaudible]--wouldn't there be extinction after a while?
[inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:
Good question. The discussion was over using
things like poker chips for reinforcement and the point is
exactly right. Since the connection with the
poker chips is established through classical conditioning,
sooner or later by that logic the poker chips would lose their
power to serve as reinforcers. You'd have to sort of start it
up again, retrain again. If you have a dog and you say
"Good dog" to reward the dog, by your logic,
which is right, at some point you might as well
give the dog a treat along with the "Good dog."
Otherwise, "Good dog" is not going to cut it anymore.
Yes. Student:
[inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:
As far as I know, Skinner and Skinnerian
psychologists were never directly involved in the
creation of prisons. On the other hand,
the psychological theory of behaviorism has had a huge
impact and I think a lot of people's ways of thinking about
criminal justice and criminal law has been shaped by
behaviorist principles. So for instance,
institutions like mental institutions and some prisons
have installed token economies where there's rewards for good
behavior, often poker chips of a sort.
And then you could cash them in for other things.
And, to some extent, these have been shaped by an
adherence to behaviorist principles.
Okay. So, here are the three general
positions of behaviorism. (1) That there is no innate
knowledge. All you need is learning.
(2) That you could explain human psychology without mental
notions like desires and goals. (3) And that these mechanisms
apply across all domains and across all species.
I think it's fair to say that right now just about everybody
agrees all of these three claims are mistaken.
First, we know that it's not true that everything is learned.
There is considerable evidence for different forms of innate
knowledge and innate desires and we'll look--and we'll talk about
it in detail when we look at case studies like language
learning, the development of sexual
preference, the developing understanding of material
objects. There's a lot of debate over
how much is innate and what the character of the built-in mental
systems are but there's nobody who doubts nowadays that a
considerable amount for humans and other animals is built-in.
Is it true that talking about mental states is unscientific?
Nobody believes this anymore either.
Science, particularly more advanced sciences like physics
or chemistry, are all about unobservables.
They're all about things you can't see.
And it makes sense to explain complex and intelligent behavior
in terms of internal mechanisms and internal representations.
Once again, the computer revolution has served as an
illustrative case study. If you have a computer that
plays chess and you want to explain how the computer plays
chess, it's impossible to do so
without talking about the programs and mechanisms inside
the computer. Is it true that animals need
reinforcement and punishment to learn?
No, and there's several demonstrations at the time of
Skinner suggesting that they don't.
This is from a classic study by Tolman where rats were taught to
run a maze. And what they found was the
rats did fine. They learn to run a maze faster
and faster when they're regularly rewarded but they also
learn to run a maze faster and faster if they are not rewarded
at all. So the reward helps,
but the reward is in no sense necessary.
And here's a more sophisticated illustration of the same point.
Professor Paul Bloom: And this is the sort of
finding, an old finding from before most of you were born,
that was a huge embarrassment for the Skinnerian theory,
as it suggests that rats in fact had mental maps,
an internal mechanism that they used to understand the world –
entirely contrary to the behaviorist idea everything
could be explained in terms of reinforcement and punishment.
Finally, is it true that there's no animal-specific
constraints for learning? And again, the answer seems to
be "no." Animals, for instance,
have natural responses. So, you could train a pigeon to
peck for food but that's because pecking for food is a very
natural response. It's very difficult to train it
to peck to escape a situation. You can train it to flap its
wings to escape a situation but it's very difficult to get it to
flap its wings for food. And the idea is they have sort
of natural responses that these learning situations might
exploit and might channel, but essentially,
they do have certain natural ways of acting towards the
world. We know that not all stimuli
and responses are created equal. So, the Gray textbook has a
very nice discussion of the Garcia effect.
And the Garcia effect goes like this.
Does anybody here have any food aversions?
I don't mean foods you don't like.
I mean foods that really make you sick.
Often food aversions in humans and other animals can be formed
through a form of association. What happens is suppose you
have the flu and you get very nauseous and then at the same
point you eat some sashimi for the first time.
The connection between being nauseous and eating a new food
is very potent. And even if you know
intellectually full well that the sashimi isn't why you became
nauseous, still you'll develop an aversion to this new food.
When I was younger – when I was a teenager – I drank this
Greek liqueur, ouzo, with beer.
I didn't have the flu at the time but I became violently ill.
And as a result I cannot abide the smell of that Greek liqueur.
Now, thank God it didn't develop into an aversion to beer
but-- [laughter] Small miracles.
But the smell is very distinctive and for me--was new
to me. And so, through the Garcia
effect I developed a strong aversion.
What's interesting though is the aversion is special so if
you take an animal and you give it a new food and then you give
it a drug to make it nauseous it will avoid that food.
But if you take an animal and you give it a new food and then
you shock it very painfully it won't avoid the new food.
And the idea is that a connection between what
something tastes and getting sick is natural.
We are hard wired to say, "Look. If I'm going to eat a new food
and I'm going to get nauseous, I'm going to avoid that food."
The Garcia effect is that this is special to taste and nausea.
It doesn't extend more generally.
Finally, I talked about phobias and I'll return to phobias later
on in this course. But the claim that people have
formed their phobias through classical conditioning is almost
always wrong. Instead, it turns out that
there are certain phobias that we're specially evolved to have.
So, both humans and chimpanzees, for instance,
are particularly prone to develop fears of snakes.
And when we talk about the emotions later on in the course
we'll talk about this in more detail.
But what seems likely is the sort of phobias you're likely to
have does not have much to do with your personal history but
rather it has a lot to do with your evolutionary history.
Finally, the other reading you're going to do for this
part--section of the course is Chomsky's classic article,
his "Review of Verbal Behavior."
Chomsky is one of the most prominent intellectuals alive.
He's still a professor at MIT, still publishes on language and
thought, among other matters. And the excerpt you're going to
read is from his "Review of Verbal Behavior."
And this is one of the most influential intellectual
documents ever written in psychology because it took the
entire discipline of behaviorism and,
more than everything else, more than any other event,
could be said to have destroyed it or ended it as a dominant
intellectual endeavor. And Chomsky's argument is
complicated and interesting, but the main sort of argument
he had to make is--goes like this.
When it comes to humans, the notions of reward and
punishment and so on that Skinner tried to extend to
humans are so vague it's not science anymore.
And remember the discussion we had with regard to Freud.
What Skinner--What Chomsky is raising here is the concern of
unfalsifiablity. So, here's the sort of example
he would discuss. Skinner, in his book Verbal
Behavior, talks about the question of why
do we do things like talk to ourselves,
imitate sounds, create art, give bad news to an
enemy, fantasize about pleasant situations?
And Skinner says that they all involve reinforcement;
those are all reinforced behaviors.
But Skinner doesn't literally mean that when we talk to
ourselves somebody gives us food pellets.
He doesn't literally mean even that when we talk to ourselves
somebody pats us on the head and says, "Good man.
Perfect. I'm very proud."
What he means, for instance,
in this case is well, talking to yourself is
self-reinforcing or giving bad news to an enemy is reinforcing
because it makes your enemy feel bad.
Well, Chomsky says the problem is not that that's wrong.
That's all true. It's just so vague as to be
useless. Skinner isn't saying anything
more. To say giving bad news to an
enemy is reinforcing because it makes the enemy feel bad doesn't
say anything different from giving bad news to an enemy
feels good because we like to give bad news to an enemy.
It's just putting it in more scientific terms.
More generally, Chomsky suggests that the law
of effect when applied to humans is either trivially true,
trivially or uninterestingly true, or scientifically robust
and obviously false. So, if you want to expand the
notion of reward or reinforcement to anything,
then it's true. So why did you come--those of
you who are not freshmen--Oh, you--Why did you come?
All of you, why did you come to Yale for a second semester?
"Well, I repeated my action because the first semester was
rewarding." Okay.
What do you mean by that? Well, you don't literally mean
that somebody rewarded you, gave you pellets and stuff.
What you mean is you chose to come there for the second
semester. And there's nothing wrong with
saying that but we shouldn't confuse it with science.
And more generally, the problem is you can talk
about what other people do in terms of reinforcement and
punishment and operant conditioning and classical
conditioning. But in order to do so,
you have to use terms like "punishment" and "reward" and
"reinforcement" in such a vague way that in the end you're not
saying anything scientific. So, behaviorism as a dominant
intellectual field has faded, but it still leaves behind an
important legacy and it still stands as one of the major
contributions of twentieth century psychology.
For one thing, it has given us a richer
understanding of certain learning mechanisms,
particularly with regard to nonhumans.
Mechanisms like habituation, classical conditioning and
operant conditioning are real; they can be scientifically
studied; and they play an important role
in the lives of animals and probably an important role in
human lives as well. They just don't explain
everything. Finally, and this is something
I'm going to return to on Wednesday actually,
behaviorists have provided powerful techniques for training
particularly for nonverbal creatures so this extends to
animal trainers. But it also extends to people
who want to teach young children and babies and also want to help
populations like the severely autistic or the severely
retarded. Many of these behaviorist
techniques have proven to be quite useful.
And in that regard, as well as in other regards,
it stands as an important contribution.
Mobile Analytics