1. Po raz pierwszy odwiedzasz EDU. LEARN

    Odwiedzasz EDU.LEARN

    Najlepszym sposobem na naukę języka jest jego używanie. W EDU.LEARN znajdziesz interesujące teksty i videa, które dadzą Ci taką właśnie możliwość. Nie przejmuj się - nasze filmiki mają napisy, dzięki którym lepiej je zrozumiesz. Dodatkowo, po kliknięciu na każde słówko, otrzymasz jego tłumaczenie oraz prawidłową wymowę.

    Nie, dziękuję
  2. Mini lekcje

    Podczas nauki języka bardzo ważny jest kontekst. Zdjęcia, przykłady użycia, dialogi, nagrania dźwiękowe - wszystko to pomaga Ci zrozumieć i zapamiętać nowe słowa i wyrażenia. Dlatego stworzyliśmy Mini lekcje. Są to krótkie lekcje, zawierające kontekstowe slajdy, które zwiększą efektywność Twojej nauki. Są cztery typy Mini lekcji - Gramatyka, Dialogi, Słówka i Obrazki.

    Dalej
  3. Wideo

    Ćwicz język obcy oglądając ciekawe filmiki. Wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na filmik. Nie martw się, obok każdego z nich są napisy. A może wcale nie będą Ci one potrzebne? Spróbuj!

    Dalej
  4. Teksty

    Czytaj ciekawe artykuły, z których nauczysz się nowych słówek i dowiesz więcej o rzeczach, które Cię interesują. Podobnie jak z filmikami, możesz wybrać temat oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknąć na wybrany artykuł. Nasz interaktywny słownik pomoże Ci zrozumieć nawet trudne teksty, a kontekst ułatwi zapamiętanie słówek. Dodatkowo, każdy artykuł może być przeczytany na głos przez wirtualnego lektora, dzięki czemu ćwiczysz słuchanie i wymowę!

    Dalej
  5. Słowa

    Tutaj możesz znaleźć swoją listę "Moje słówka", czyli funkcję wyszukiwania słówek - a wkrótce także słownik tematyczny. Do listy "Moje słówka" możesz dodawać słowa z sekcji Videa i Teksty. Każde z słówek dodanych do listy możesz powtórzyć później w jednym z naszych ćwiczeń. Dodatkowo, zawsze możesz iść do swojej listy i sprawdzić znaczenie, wymowę oraz użycie słówka w zdaniu. Użyj naszej wyszukiwarki słówek w części "Słownictwo", aby znaleźć słowa w naszej bazie.

    Dalej
  6. Lista tekstów

    Ta lista tekstów pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Teksty". Wybierz poziom trudności oraz temat, a następnie artykuł, który Cię interesuje. Kiedy już zostaniesz do niego przekierowany, kliknij na "Play", jeśli chcesz, aby został on odczytany przez wirtualnego lektora. W ten sposób ćwiczysz umiejętność słuchania. Niektóre z tekstów są szczególnie interesujące - mają one odznakę w prawym górnym rogu. Koniecznie je przeczytaj!

    Dalej
  7. Lista Video

    Ta lista filmików pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Video". Podobnie jak w przypadku Tekstów, najpierw wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na wybrane video. Te z odznaką w prawym górnym rogu są szczególnie interesujące - nie przegap ich!

    Dalej
  8. Dziękujemy za skorzystanie z przewodnika!

    Teraz już znasz wszystkie funkcje EDU.LEARN! Przygotowaliśmy do Ciebie wiele artykułów, filmików oraz mini lekcji - na pewno znajdziesz coś, co Cię zainteresuje!

    Teraz zapraszamy Cię do zarejestrowania się i odkrycia wszystkich możliwości portalu.

    Dziękuję, wrócę później
  9. Lista Pomocy

    Potrzebujesz z czymś pomocy? Sprawdź naszą listę poniżej:
    Nie, dziękuję

Już 62 483 użytkowników uczy się języków obcych z Edustation.

Możesz zarejestrować się już dziś i odebrać bonus w postaci 10 monet.

Jeżeli chcesz się dowiedzieć więcej o naszym portalu - kliknij tutaj

Jeszcze nie teraz

lub

Poziom:

Wszystkie

Nie masz konta?

Charles Limb: Your brain on improv


Poziom:

Temat: Nauka i technologia

So I am a surgeon who studies creativity,
and I have never had a patient tell me
that "I really want you to be creative during surgery."
And so I guess there's a little bit of irony to it.
I will say though that, after having done surgery a lot,
it's somewhat similar to playing a musical instrument.
And for me, this sort of deep and enduring fascination with sound
is what led me to both be a surgeon
and also to study the science of sound, particularly music.
And so I'm going to try to talk to you over the next few minutes
about my career
in terms of how I'm able to actually try to study music
and really try to grapple with these questions
of how is the brain able to be creative.
I've done most of this work at Johns Hopkins University,
but also at the National Institute of Health where I was previously.
I'm going to go over some science experiments
and try to cover three musical experiments.
I'm going to start off by playing a video for you.
And this video is a video of Keith Jarrett, who's a well-know jazz improviser
and probably the most well-known, iconic example
of someone who takes improvisation to a really higher level.
And he'll improvise entire concerts
off the top of his head,
and he'll never play it exactly the same way again.
And so, as a form of intense creativity,
I think this is a great example.
And so why don't we go and click the video.
(Music)
It's really a remarkable, awesome thing that happens there.
I've always -- just as a listener, as just a fan --
I listen to that, and I'm just astounded.
I think: how can this possibly be?
How can the brain generate that much information,
that much music spontaneously?
And so I set out with this concept, scientifically,
that artistic creativity, it's magical, but it's not magic.
Meaning that it's a product from the brain.
There's not too many brain-dead people creating art.
And so with this notion that artistic creativity
is in fact a neurologic product,
I took this thesis that we could study it
just like we can study any other complex neurologic process.
And I think there's some sub-questions there that I put there.
Is it truly possible to study creativity scientifically?
And I think that's a good question.
And I'll tell you that most scientific studies of music,
they're very dense.
And when you go through them, it's very hard to recognize the music in it.
In fact, they seem to be very unmusical entirely
and to miss the whole point of the music.
And so it brings the second question:
Why should scientists study creativity?
Maybe we're not the right people to do it.
Well it may be,
but I will say that, from a scientific perspective --
we talked a lot about innovation today --
the science of innovation,
how much we understand about how the brain is able to innovate
is in its infancy.
Truly, we know very little about how we are able to be creative.
And so I think that we're going to see
over the next 10, 20, 30 years
a real science of creativity that's burgeoning and is going to flourish.
Because we now have new methods that can enable us
to take this process of something like this,
complex jazz improvisation, and study it rigorously.
And so it gets down to the brain.
And so all of us have this remarkable brain,
which is poorly understood to say the least.
I think that neuroscientists
have much more questions than answers.
And I myself, I'm not going to give you many answers today,
just ask a lot of questions.
And fundamentally that's what I do in my lab.
I ask questions about what is this brain doing to enable us to do this.
This is the main method that I use. This is called functional MRI.
If you've been in an MRI scanner, it's very much the same,
but this one is outfitted in a special way
to not just take pictures of your brain,
but to also take pictures of active areas of the brain.
Now the way that's done is by the following.
There's something called BOLD imaging,
which is blood oxygen level imaging.
Now when you're in an fMRI scanner,
you're in a big magnet
that's aligning your molecules in certain areas.
When an area of the brain is active, meaning a neural area is active,
it gets blood flow shunted to that area.
That blood flow causes an increase
in local blood to that area
with a deoxyhemoglobin change in concentration.
Deoxyhemoglobin can be detected by MRI,
whereas oxyhemoglobin can't.
So through this method of inference --
and we're measuring blood flow, not neural activity --
we say that an area of the brain that's getting more blood
was active during a particular task.
And that's the crux of how fMRI works.
And it's been used since the 90's
to study really complex processes.
Now I'm going to review a study that I did,
which was Jazz in an fMRI scanner.
And this was done with a colleague of mine, Alan Braun, at the NIH.
This is a short video of how we did this project.
(Video) Charles Limb: This is a plastic MIDI piano keyboard
that we use for the jazz experiments.
And it's a 35-key keyboard
that is designed to fit both inside the scanner,
be both magnetically safe,
have minimal interference
that would contribute to any artifact
and have this cushion so that it can rest on the players' legs
while they're lying down in the scanner, playing on their back.
And it works like this -- this doesn't actually produce any sound.
It sends out what's called a MIDI signal --
or a musical instrument digital interface --
through these wires into the box and then the computer,
which then trigger high-quality piano samples like this.
(Music)
(Music)
CL: Okay, so it works.
And so through this piano keyboard,
we now have the means to take a musical process and study it.
So what do you do now that you have this cool piano keyboard?
You can't just sort of -- "It's great we've got this keyboard."
We actually have to come up with a scientific experiment.
And so the experiment really rests on the following.
What happens in the brain during something that's memorized and over-learned,
and what happens in the brain during something
that is spontaneously generated, or improvised,
in a way that's matched motorically
and in terms of lower-level sensory motor features?
And so, I have here what we call the paradigms.
There's a scale paradigm, which is just playing a scale up and down, memorized.
And then there's improvising on a scale --
quarter notes, metronome, right hand --
scientifically very safe,
but musically really boring.
And then there's the bottom one, which is called the jazz paradigm.
And so what we did was we brought professional jazz players to the NIH,
and we had them memorize this piece of music on the left, the lower-left --
which is what you heard me playing --
and then we had them improvise to the same exact chord changes.
And if you can hit that lower-right sound icon,
that's an example of what was recorded in the scanner.
(Music)
So in the end, it's not the most natural environment,
but they're able to play real music.
And I've listened to that solo 200 times,
and I still like it.
And the musicians, they were comfortable in the end.
And so we first measured the number of notes.
Were they in fact just playing a lot more notes when they were improvising?
That was not what was going on.
And then we looked at the brain activity.
I'm going to try to condense this for you.
These are contrast maps that are showing subtractions
between what changes when you're improvising
versus when you're doing something memorized.
In red is an area that active in the prefrontal cortex,
the frontal lobe of the brain.
And in blue is this area that was deactivated.
And so we had this focal area called the medial prefrontal cortex
that went way up in activity.
We had this broad patch of area called the lateral prefrontal cortex
that went way down in activity, and I'll summarize that for you here.
Now these are multifunctional areas of the brain.
As I like to say, these are not the jazz areas of the brain.
They do a whole host of things
that have to do with self-reflection,
introspection, working memory and so forth.
Really consciousness is seated in the frontal lobe.
But we have this combination
of an area that's thought to be involved in self-monitoring, turning off,
and this area that's thought to be autobiographical,
or self-expressive, turning on.
And we think, at least in this preliminary --
It's one study. It's probably wrong.
But it's one study.
We think that at least a reasonable hypothesis
is that, to be creative,
you have to have this weird dissociation in your frontal lobe.
One area turns on, and a big area shuts off,
so that you're not inhibited, so that you're willing to make mistakes,
so that you're not constantly shutting down
all of these new generative impulses.
Now a lot of people know that music is not always a solo activity --
sometimes it's done communicatively.
And so the next question was:
What happens when musicians are trading back and forth,
something called trading fours,
which is something they do normally in a jazz experiment?
So this is a twelve-bar blues.
And I've broken it down into four-bar groups here,
so you would know how you would trade.
Now what we did was we brought a musician into the scanner -- same way --
had them memorize this melody
and then had another musician out in the control room
trading back and forth interactively.
So this is a musician, Mike Pope,
one of the world's best bassists and a fantastic piano player.
So he's now playing the piece
that we just saw
just a little better than I wrote it.
(Video) CL: Mike, come on in. (Man: May the force be with you.)
Nurse: Nothing's in your pockets, right Mike?
Mike Pope: Nope. Nothing's in my pockets. (Nurse: Okay.)
CL: You have to have the right attitude to agree to it.
(Laughter)
It's kind of fun actually.
And so now we're playing back and forth.
He's in there. You can see his legs up there.
And then I'm in the control room here, playing back and forth.
(Music)
(Video) Mike Pope: This is a pretty good representation
of what it's like.
And it's good that it's not too quick.
The fact that we do it over and over again
lets you acclimate to your surroundings.
So the hardest thing for me was the kinesthetic thing,
of looking at my hands
through two mirrors,
laying on my back
and not able to move at all except for my hand.
That was challenging.
But again,
there were moments, for sure,
there were moments
of real, honest-to-God musical interplay, for sure.
CL: At this point, I'll take a few moments.
And so what you're seeing here --
And I'm doing a cardinal sin in science,
which is to show you preliminary data.
This is one subject's data.
This is, in fact, Mike Pope's data.
So what am I showing you here?
When he was trading fours with me, improvising versus memorized,
his language areas lit up, his Broca's area,
which is inferior frontal gyrus on the left.
He actually had it also homologous on the right.
This is an area thought to be involved in expressive communication.
This whole notion that music is a language,
well maybe there's a neurologic basis to it in fact after all,
and we can see it when two musicians are having a musical conversation.
And so we've done actually this on eight subjects now,
and we're just getting all the data together.
So hopefully we'll have something to say it meaningfully.
Now when I think about improvisation and the language, well what's next?
Rap, of course, rap --
free-style.
And so I've always been fascinated by free-style.
And let's go ahead and play this video here.
(Video) Mos Def: ♫ ... brown skin I be, standing five-ten I be ♫
♫ Rockin' it when I be, in your vicinity ♫
♫ Whole-style synergy, recognize symmetry ♫
♫ Go and try to injure me, broke 'em down chemically ♫
♫ Ain't the number 10 M.C., talk about how been I be ♫
♫ Styled it like Kennedy, late like a 10 to three ♫
♫ When I say when I be, girls say bend that key cut ♫
CL: And so there's a lot of analogy
between what takes place in free-style rap and jazz.
There are, in fact, a lot of correlus between the two forms of music
I think in different time periods.
In a lot a ways, rap serves the same social function
that jazz used to serve.
So how do you study rap scientifically?
And my colleagues kind of think I'm crazy,
but I think it's very viable.
And so this is what you do: you have a free-style artist
come in and memorize a rap that you write for them,
that they've never heard before,
and then you have them free-style.
So I told my lab members that I would rap for TED,
and they said, "No, you won't."
And then I thought --
(Applause)
But here's the thing.
With this big screen, you can all rap with me. Okay?
So what we had them do
was memorize this lower-left sound icon please.
This is the control condition. This is what they memorized.
Computer: ♫ Memory, thump. ♫
CL: ♫ Thump of the beat in a known repeat ♫
♫ Rhythm and rhyme, they make me complete ♫
♫ The climb is sublime when I'm on the mic ♫
♫ Spittin' rhymes that hit you like a lightning strike ♫
♫ I search for the truth in this eternal quest ♫
♫ My passion's not fashion, you can see how I'm dressed ♫
♫ Psychopathic words, in my head appear ♫
♫ Whisper these lyrics only I can hear ♫
♫ The art of discovering and that which is hovering ♫
♫ Inside the mind of those unconfined ♫
♫ All of these words keep pouring out like rain ♫
♫ I need a mad scientist to check my brain ♫
(Applause)
I guarantee you, that will never happen again.
(Laughter)
So now, what's great about these free-stylers,
they will get cued different words.
They don't know what's coming, but they'll hear something off the cuff.
Go ahead and hit that right sound icon.
They are going to be cued these three square words: 'like', 'not' and 'head'.
He doesn't know what's coming.
Free-styler: ♫ I'm like some kind of [unclear] ♫
♫ [unclear] extraterrestrial, celestial scene ♫
♫ Back in the days, I used to sit in pyramids and meditate ♫
♫ With two microphones hovering over my head ♫
♫ See if I could still listen, spittin' off the sound ♫
♫ See what you grinning ♫
♫ I teach the children in the back of the classroom ♫
♫ About the message of apocalyptical ♫
♫ Not really though, cuz I've got to keep it simple ♫
♫ [unclear] instrumental ♫
♫ Detrimental playing Super Mario ♫
♫ [unclear] boxes [unclear] hip hop ♫
CL: So again, it's an incredible thing that's taking place.
It's doing something that, neurologically, is remarkable.
Whether or not you like the music is irrelevant.
Creatively speaking, it's just a phenomenal thing.
This is a short video of how we actually do this in a scanner.
(Laughter)
(Video) CL: We're here with Emmanuel.
CL: That was recorded in the scanner, by the way.
(Video) CL: That's Emmanuel in the scanner.
He's just memorized a rhyme for us.
Emmanuel: ♫ Top of the beat with no repeat ♫
♫ Rhythm and rhyme make me complete ♫
♫ Climb is sublime when I'm on the mic ♫
♫ Spittin' rhymes that'll hit you like a lightning strike ♫
♫ I search for the truth in this eternal quest ♫
♫ I'm passing on fashion; you can see how I'm dressed ♫
CL: Okay. So I'm going to stop that there. So what do we see in his brain?
Well, this is actually four rappers' brains.
And what we see, we do see language areas lighting up,
but then -- eyes closed --
when you are free-styling versus memorizing,
you've got major visual areas lighting up.
You've got major cerebellar activity, which is involved in motor coordination.
You have heightened brain activity when you're doing a comparable task,
when that one task is creative and the other task is memorized.
It's very preliminary, but I think it's kind of cool.
So just to conclude, we've got a lot of questions to ask.
And like I said, we'll ask questions here, not answer them.
But we want to get at the root of what is creative genius, neurologically.
And I think, with these methods, we're getting close to being there.
And I think hopefully in the next 10, 20 years
you'll actually see real, meaningful studies
that say science has to catch up to art,
and maybe we're starting now to get there.
And so I want to thank you for your time. I appreciate it.
(Applause)
Mobile Analytics